Find Local Events (pick a date)
 
Browse Boulder real estate by neighborhood, school and zip code along with other homes for sale in Colorado on COhomefinder.com
Browse Boulder real estate by neighborhood, school and zip code along with other homes for sale in Colorado on COhomefinder.com.

 

 

 

 

 

 

 
Home / Articles / Entertainment / Books /  Natural-born talent? Author sheds new light on nature-nurture debate
. . . . . . .
Give Through iGivefirst
Thursday, April 22,2010

Natural-born talent? Author sheds new light on nature-nurture debate

By Adam Perry

Like most Americans, I was given an IQ test in the first grade and was told I fell just short of what my teachers and parents called “gifted.” Several times a week from elementary school through high school, the students labeled “gifted” were separated from the rest of us to be given extra attention, extra praise, more diverse and interesting schoolwork, and even a better teacher.

To the adults I met as a child, a person’s capacity for achievement could be decided at around age 6 or 7 by a simple equation: genes environment, or “nature nurture.” Given an extra dose of more expansive learning by dynamic teachers, they thought, a “gifted” child could metaphorically grow from a caterpillar into a butterfly. But they were convinced that the rest of us are eternally mired in the sad-but-true stasis known as being average, wholly incapable of growing wings no matter the effort or interest.

Here’s a shocker: Some students labeled “gifted” go on to impressive success and some do not, just like the rest of us. So how does a childhood overachiever, constantly heaped with praise, end up dropping out of college and working in a fast-food joint? How does a troubled high school dropout who never stepped foot in a university become a renowned artist and intellectual whose lectures are treasured worldwide?

David Shenk, author of the best-selling new book The Genius in All of Us, has the answer, and it’s a simple one: nature multiplied by nurture.

“No one is genetically bound to mediocrity,” Shenk writes. In fact, even Alfred Binet (inventor of the original IQ test) insisted that we “protest” against the idea that “intelligence is a fixed quantity.” Instead, our genes interact with our environment, and the success of our scholarly, creative and professional evolution is directly related to the cultural exposure we’re afforded and the demands put on us by our environment and ourselves.

“It’s pretty easy in life to take a mediocre path,” Shenk told Boulder Weekly in a recent interview. “Bad parenting can get us there pretty quickly. So can lousy schools and/ or a limited sense of one’s own potential. But the possibilities of achievement are never fully cut off. We remain adaptable throughout our lives.”

Robert C. Brown, who founded the contemplative education department at Naropa University in 1990, agrees.

“The learning environment is a key element in developing intelligence in the broadest sense of the word,” Brown says. “It’s a matter of expanding our learning relationships. What we mean by intelligence or genius has been widened in recent times … but we don’t know how to recognize it, and usually our teachers don’t. Part of the reason for that is that we haven’t learned how to actually observe or contemplate individual genius. … If we [don’t] recognize it, we don’t really know how to cultivate it.”

In a 1958 lab study documented in The Genius in All of Us, psychologists at the University of Manitoba raised two genetic strains of rats (labeled “dull” and “bright”) in three distinct environments — “enriched,” “normal” and “restricted” (or devoid of stimulation). Expecting to prove that, as Shenk wrote, “each strain would get a little smarter in the enriched environment and a little dumber in the restricted environment,” the results turned out to be astonishing.

The “dull” and “bright” rats — genetically consistent over many generations — lived up to their genetic brands under “normal” conditions but performed almost exactly the same as each other when raised in an environment rich with stimulation or condemned to a boredom-filled upbringing.

Not surprisingly, the game-changing revelation that Shenk calls “the geneenvironment interactive dynamic” applies to humans as well. A child labeled “average” and simultaneously confined to cultural isolation in a place where edifying books, art, teachers and experiences are all but foreign can easily fade into the indistinguishable masses of unremarkable individuals. But, given the same chance as those we label “gifted” — and a whole lot of what Shenk describes as “deliberate practice” (at least 10,000 hours of focused, singular activity in which our previous limits are painfully stretched) — almost anyone can flourish.

The work has to start early and almost never cease. Think of people like Mozart and baseball legend Ted Williams — accomplished heroes we regularly portray as having “God-given talent.” With Williams, Mozart, Michael Jordan and Yo-Yo Ma as examples, Shenk asserts that what we call “talent” does not exist. Instead, deliberate practice (the crux of Shenk’s idea of how to cultivate genius) functions in tandem with motivation and stimulation (plus the genes we’re given) to create magnificence in everything from mathematics to basketball.

In short, with the exception of aberrations such as autism, geniuses are “trained,” not born.

Still, none of this new information suggests that schools should stop giving IQ tests to children. However, perhaps we should stop telling children that their IQ circa grade school represents their promise in life, or even their prospective IQ.

“Humans are not assigned, by genetic code or by IQ test performance, to achievement levels — learning is a matter of participating within social and cultural systems, not bubbling in the correct circle on a test,” says University of Colorado professor Noah Finkelstein, a leading researcher in studies of student learning in physics and science.

Finkelstein lamented that “our society builds expectations into the way we test and sort students,” adding that “these expectations do not match nor reflect the potential capacities of our students.”

Shenk concurs. “I don’t think we have to replace the test itself,” he says. “The test works fine. It’s how we talk about the test that’s the problem. We need to make sure that parents and kids understand that tests reflect current abilities — they do not reveal one’s inner potential.”

That goes for those labeled “gifted” early on, too.

“Childhood achievers are frequently hobbled by the psychology of their own success,” Shenk writes in a chapter about prodigies who unravel after childhoods spent in the bosom of “praise for being technically proficient at a specific task.” Prodigies often “develop natural aversion to stepping outside their comfort zone,” manifesting the “terrible fear of new challenges and of any sort of flaw or failure.” Thus, post-childhood success can be thwarted, partly due to impenetrable social walls constructed in seclusion amid the effort to sustain acclaim from adults who recognize them only as a “whiz kid.”

Getting back to that “promising beginning” can be excruciating.

“It certainly can be difficult for a once-prodigy to find his or her way in adulthood,” Shenk says, “and that includes maintaining a passion for one’s craft. Part of it involves a new understanding of achievement as a collection of skills, rather than innate giftedness.”

Shenk’s diversely impressive career trajectory — which has seen him shift from co-authoring a cult-classic on the Grateful Dead to lecturing on information technology and penning a hugely successful book on Alzheimer’s that became a PBS film — also reveals a writer and thinker always eager to remain both iconoclastic and interested. Surely our educational systems could encourage this kind of far-reaching scholarship from kindergarten on.

Says Naropa’s Brown, “When we are educating for only a narrow band-width of intelligence, it becomes very fragile and unsustainable. When we start thinking very broadly about the meaning of education, then individual excellence starts to manifest.”

It’s obviously harmful in many ways for children who are praised for proficiency in one area to be withheld from meaningful opportunities to grow in what Finkelstein calls “a rich mixture of individual and environment.” But how fortunate that each of us has the chance to survive suffocating social and cultural structures and become a whiz kid at any age.

Respond: letters@boulderweekly.com

  • Currently 3.5/5 Stars.
  • 1
  • 2
  • 3
  • 4
  • 5
POST A COMMENT
No Registration Required
 
Close
Close